Genetics and Leadership The Determinants of Leadership: The Role of Genetic, Personality, and Cognitive Factors

نویسندگان

  • Richard D. Arvey
  • Maria Rotundo
  • Wendy Johnson
  • Matt McGue
چکیده

A sample of 646 male twins (331 monozygotic or identical, 315 dizygotic or fraternal twins) completed a survey indicating their leadership role occupancy in work settings. Data on these individuals were also available for personality and cognitive variables. As predicted, two personality variables (Social Potency and Achievement) and a cognitive variable (a vocabulary test) were significantly correlated with the leadership variable. Subsequently, univariate and multivariate genetic analyses showed that a substantial portion of this leadership variance was accounted for by genetic factors (39 percent) while non-shared (or non-common) environmental factors accounted for the remaining variance in this leadership variable. Genetic influences were shown for the personality and cognitive factors as well. Finally, results indicated that the genetic influences for the leadership factor were substantially associated with or common with the genetic factors influencing the personality factors but not with the cognitive variable. Genetics and Leadership 2 The Determinants of Leadership: The Role of Genetics, Personality, and Cognitive Variables What are the determinants of leadership in work and organizational settings? This question has been pursued for decades. Throughout the years, a variety of constructs and predictors have been posited as determinants of leadership including general intelligence, personality, values, and even genetic factors. Though the proposition that individual differences or “traits” can predict and/or explain differences in emergent or effective leadership has sometimes been viewed with skepticism, current research has more firmly established the robustness of these types of variables in predicting leadership criteria. For example, Judge, Bono, Illies, and Gehrardt (2002) present the results of their metaanalysis showing that personality variables are consistently and reliably correlated with leadership variables, Chan and Drasgow (2001) demonstrate that a number of cognitive, personality, and motivational constructs are related to leadership across samples from different international environments, and Schneider, Paul, White, and Holcome (1999) show that a variety of constructs drawn from personality, interests, and motivation domains predicts leadership among high school students. Because of the firm foundation regarding the relationships between the constructs of individual differences and leadership, it is not far-fetched to ask whether leadership is genetically influenced. Indeed, the notion that leadership has genetic influences has been articulated in practitioner and scholarly articles over the years. For example, in a recent Harvard Business Review article, Sorcher and Brant (2002) say: “Our experience has led us to believe that much of leadership talent is hardwired in people before they reach their Genetics and Leadership 3 early or mid-twenties” (p. 81). In contrast, Kellaway (2002) reports the efforts of a major US Bank to develop all of its employees (95,000 of them) into leaders, reflecting the belief that leadership is entirely under developmental influences. It is interesting to note that almost no research exists that examines this “nature-nurture” issue using a contemporary behavior genetics research design, even though Bass (1990, p. 911) and Arvey and Bouchard (1994, p. 70) suggest that such analyses would be quite appropriate. In addition, Arvey and Bouchard (1994) indicate that while there may be evidence for genetic influences on variables like leadership, such relationships are most likely mediated through other intermediate constructs (i.e. psychological and physiological variables). The current study explores the relationships of different personality and cognitive constructs with leadership as well as the roles genetic influences play in these associations. Background: Several literature bases are important in developing the model and objectives for this study. First, the research base establishing a genetic basis for leadership is limited. To our knowledge, only one previous study has examined this issue. Johnson, Vernon, McCarthy, Molson, Harris and Jang (1998) report the results of a study using 183 MZ and 64 DZ same-sex twin pairs. The Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ ; Bass & Avolio, 1991) and other leadership measures (e.g. adjective checklist items) were completed by these twins. Two factors resembling transactional and transformational leadership dimensions were derived from MLQ items by factor analytic procedures. Results indicated that 48% and 59% of the variance in the transactional and transformational leadership dimensions respectively was associated with genetic factors. Genetics and Leadership 4 The data also indicated that the genetic factor for the transformational dimension reflected a non-additive or dominant effect—that is, the impact of one gene depends on influence of another instead of simply “adding up”. Other analyses showed that there were common genetic factors in the covariance found between these two leadership dimensions from the MLQ and several other leadership scales. This is an important entry into the research issue of whether leadership has some genetic associations. We expand on this research in several ways. First, we incorporate alternative measures of leadership that focus on leadership role occupancy that are perhaps more clearly distinguishable from other measured facets of leadership style. Second, we incorporate an expanded model proposing and testing a model of the determinants of leadership proposing that cognitive and personality variables are related to role occupancy (see below). This is important because simply showing that a construct is heritable leaves many unanswered questions regarding how the genetic mechanisms work and through which processes. Moreover, we investigate whether and to what degree any observed relationships between personality and cognitive variables and our leadership variable are due to common genetic factors. A second literature base has to do with the research demonstrating relationships between personality dimensions and leadership. While a number of studies have demonstrated that personality variables are useful in predicting various aspects of job performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991, Hough, 1992; Lord, DeVader, & Alliger, 1986), there is also evidence that such variables predict a variety of leadership criteria. As mentioned above, Judge, Bono, Ilies, and Gerhardt (2002) meta-analyzed 222 correlations from 73 samples providing personality data according to the five-factor Genetics and Leadership 5 model (Digman, 1990) and found that measures of Extraversion correlated .31, measures of Conscientiousness correlated .28, measures of Openness correlated .24, and measures of Neuroticism correlated -.24 with leadership emergence (after corrections for unreliability but not range restriction). Similar findings have been reported previously by Hogan, Curphy, and Hogan (1994), Yukl (1998), Bass (1990), and Daft (1999). Thus, there is a substantial research base establishing a link between personality variables and leadership. In addition, the genetic basis for personality is well established dating back to Loehlin and Nichols (1976). Since then similar results have been obtained for a variety of personality measures. A few examples include the study by Jang, Livesley, and Vernon (1996) who used a twin methodology where 123 pairs of monzygotic (MZ) twins and 121 dizygotic (DZ) twins were assessed using the revised NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). This inventory is used to capture the five factors of personality mentioned above. The estimates of genetic influence (or heritabilities) of these dimensions were as follows: Neuroticism (41%), Extraversion (53%), Openness (61%), Agreeableness (41%), and Conscientiousness (44%). Using twin pairs (about 800) drawn from the National Merit Twin Study, Loehlin, McCrae, Costa, and John (1998) showed that the “Big Five” personality factors were substantially and comparably heritable with about 50% of the variance in these personality constructs being associated with genetic factors; however little or no influence due to shared family environment was found among these twin pairs. Similar estimates were obtained by Rieman, Angleitner, and Strelau (1997) using twin samples recruited for their study in Germany. Rowe (1994) summarizes his own earlier study (Loehlin and Rowe, 1992) where multiple studies and Genetics and Leadership 6 samples were analyzed which differed in terms of their genetic relationships (e.g. twins, parent-child, adoptive siblings, etc.) as well as other sample characteristics (e.g. different age groups, different geographical areas, etc.). The heritability estimates for the big five personality dimensions ranged from .39 to .49, with the heritability for Extraversion demonstrating the highest estimate (.49). Rowe concluded that “Individuals who share genes are alike in personality regardless of how they are reared, whereas rearing environment induces little or no personality resemblance” (Rowe, 1994, p. 64). Moving beyond personality measures relying on the Five Factor taxonomy, Tellegen, Lykken, Bouchard, Wilcox, Segal, and Rich (1988) report a study using twins who were assessed on the 11 major personality traits as measured by the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire (MPQ, Tellegen, 1982). Their data indicated that genetic influences were significant and substantial for all 11 scales (ranging from .39 for achievement to .58 for constraint). For excellent contemporary reviews affirming the heritabilities of personality traits see Bouchard (1997) and Bouchard and Loehlin (2001). Finally, there is a research base showing that measures of intelligence are correlated with leadership variables. For example, meta-analytic results reported by Lord, DeVader, and Alliger (1986) indicate that the mean true correlation of measures of intelligence and leadership is .50. Other reviews by Stogdill (1974), Bray and Howard (1983) and others support the intelligence—leadership relationship. More recently, Schneider, Ehrhart, and Ehrhart (in press) showed that one of the most consistent predictors of leadership in high school students was grade point average, a typical proxy of mental ability. There is also a robust research base demonstrating that general cognitive ability is heritable (Bouchard & McGue, 1981) where approximately 50 percent Genetics and Leadership 7 of the total variance of such constructs can be accounted for by genetic factors and about one quarter by shared environmental factors (Plomin, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2001; Plomin and Rende, 1991). Thus, the inference that cognitive ability will be related to leadership and that a genetic component might underlie this relationship is not difficult to make. Research Objectives. Given this background, the objectives of the present research are to investigate the following: 1) to affirm the relationships of a variety of personality, and cognitive ability measures with leadership, 2) to investigate the role of genetic influences in explaining these personality and mental ability constructs as well as leadership itself, and 3) to investigate the degree to which any observed relationships between these predictor constructs and leadership variables are due to common genetic influences. Method and Results Sample. The sample for this study was drawn from the Minnesota Twin Registry. The Registry is the product of an ongoing effort to locate as many as possible of the 10,000 surviving intact twin pairs born in Minnesota from 1936 to 1981 (Lykken, Bouchard, McGue, & Tellegen, 1990). The Minnesota Twin Registry subsample examined in the present study was assessed as part of the Minnesota Parenting Project, a broad study of life outcomes in men born between the years 1961 and 1964. The sample was restricted to men born in those years in order to hold age, sex, and birth cohort relatively constant. For purposes of this study, the relevant aspect of the sample was that it was representative of young working-age men born in Minnesota during this time. We sent surveys to 558 male twin pairs (1,116 individuals) who participated in this earlier study. A total of 646 completed surveys were returned, yielding a response rate of 58%. Of the 646 returned Genetics and Leadership 8 and completed surveys, 426 included both members of the twin pair. Of these 213 twin pairs (426 participants), 119 pairs were monozygotic twins and 94 pairs were dizygotic twins. As was their Minnesota birth cohort, the sample was primarily White (98%), and had an average age of 36.7 years (SD = 1.12). A total of 78% were married or living with a partner, 8% were divorced, separated, or widowed, and 14% were single. Other relevant characteristics of the total sample and twin types are presented in Table 1. The largest proportion of the sample described themselves as working in the production, construction, operating, maintenance, material handling (34.3%) or professional, paraprofessional, or technical (26.6%) occupations. No differences were observed between twin types on these variables. The participants' zygosity had been determined as part of the Minnesota Parenting Project, using a five-item questionnaire that has been shown to exceed 95% accuracy compared to serological methods for establishing zygosity (Lykken, Bouchard, McGue, and Tellegen, 1990). Measures Used. A variety of measures reflecting the different constructs were used. They are as follows: Leadership: The definition and measurement of leadership has vexed researchers for years. Yukl (1998) and others have provided an overview of the major research approaches taken. These include the: 1) power-influence approach, 2) trait approach, 3) behavioral approach, and 4) situational approach. These different approaches are well known and articulated in many sources; thus, we will not repeat here a description of these various paradigms. For the present research, we measured leadership from a leadership “emergence” perspective where leadership is defined and measured in terms of Genetics and Leadership 9 the various formal and informal leadership role attainments of individuals in work settings (see Judge, et al., [2000] for a discussion of “emergence” versus “effectiveness” leadership distinctions). We focused exclusively on leadership in work because of its relatively greater interest to behavioral scientists in this area (e.g. industrial psychologists). Our leadership measure was developed using a “bio-history” methodology where respondents indicated past participation or role occupation in leadership positions. The bio-history or biographical approach to psychological measurement is a well-known and acceptable procedure in assessing autobiographical or historical events among individuals (Mumford & Stokes, 1992), including assessments of leadership potential and effectiveness (Stricker & Rock, 1998; McElreath & Bass, 1999; Chan & Drasgow, 2001). Respondents in our study replied to several items: 1) List the work-related professional associations in which they served as a leader (m=2.23, s.d.=.58), 2) Indicate whether they had “taken charge of a special project”, 3) Indicate whether they had “planned or coordinated a special event” at work, and 4) Indicate whether they had held positions at work that would be considered managerial or supervisory in nature (a number of different options were presented, e.g., manager, supervisor, director, vice-president, etc.). Table 2 presents the sample responses to these items. Chi-square analyses revealed that the monozygotic twins had held significantly more work group and director leadership positions on the job (p. <.05) than the dizygotic twins. No other differences were observed. We developed four initial scores for each individual for leadership role occupancy. The first score was developed by assigning 7 points if he checked President Genetics and Leadership 10 (the highest ranking category), 6 points if he checked Vice-President (the next highestranking category) but not President, 5 points if he checked Manager but neither of the other 2 higher ranking categories, etc. We standardized this score. A second score was developed based on the number of leadership roles assumed in professional associations (standardized). A third score was based on whether the individual had taken charge of a special project (standardized), and a fourth score was based on whether the individual had planned a special event (also standardized). We averaged across these four variables to create our leadership composite; we had no a-priori justification for providing differential weights for the four scores. We argue that this composite represents one form of a multidimensional construct—the aggregate model--discussed by Law, Wong, and Mobley (1998). This model is described as when a composite variable is formed by algebraically summing a number of other variables conceptually related to the construct of interest; the variables are not necessarily statistically interrelated nor does the resulting composite necessarily represent an underlying latent construct. There were 3 outlying observations on this scale, which were trimmed to a value 2.00. The mean for this composite scale was .00 (s.d.=.67, n= 646) with a range between –.87 to 2.00. There was a significant mean difference (F=5.36, p<.05) between monozygotic twins (m = .059) and dizygotic twins (m = -.065), but the effect size was relatively small between these two groups (.18). The scale correlated significantly (p<.01) with scales formed using similar items where respondents reported their past leadership activities in high school (.21), college (.18), and in current community activities (.22). Other psychometric properties of this scale behaved as expected. For example, the scale was not significantly related to a number of variables for which there were no aGenetics and Leadership 11 priori expectations of a relationship (i.e., marital status, etc.), it was negatively correlated with variables for which a reverse relationship was expected (i.e. those who lived on a farm scored lower on this scale, -.10), and it was positively correlated with total household income (r=.23, p<.01)—a reasonable expectation (Kuhn & Weinberger, 2002). Finally, in an effort to verify that individuals were indeed in the leadership roles that they indicated, we conducted telephone interviews with 11 individuals who were among the top scorers on this variable. We asked them to provide additional details concerning the various roles they occupied (e.g. how many people they supervised, what kinds of responsibilities were involved, etc.) In almost every case, there was sufficient data for us to feel that the information provided was verified. Personality measures. The 198-item form of the Mutidimensional Personality Questionniare (MPQ; Tellegen, 1982; Tellegen & Waller, 2001) was administered to the larger twin population from which this sample was drawn. This inventory yields scores on 11 primary trait scales developed through factor analysis. The mean 30-day test-retest reliability is .87 for these MPQ primary scales. It is important to note that the sample (and larger population) completed this inventory as part of a separate survey six years earlier than the survey administered regarding the leadership measures described above. Thus, there was a considerable time difference between the completion of this personality inventory and the leadership survey. This reduces the possibility of inflated correlations due to same-time method bias and establishes some plausibility for the premise that personality predicts leadership rather than vice-versa. As mentioned in the introduction, the trait scales based on the MPQ scales have demonstrated relatively high heritabilities based on other samples. Also, while the trait scales from the MPQ do not map exactly Genetics and Leadership 12 onto the taxonomy provided by the Five Factor organization of personality dimensions, there is sufficient similarities between several of the dimensions and facets subsumed under the Five Factor structure (see Hough & Ones [2002} and Bouchard & Lochlin [2001] for a cross-walk between the Five Factor model and the MPQ scales and Church [1994] for an empirical examination of the relationships between MPQ scales and Costa & McCrae’s [1995] NEO Personality Inventory, which directly measures dimensions organized around the Five Factor model). We selected the three scales from the MPQ that are most relevant to leadership—Social Potency, Achievement, and Social Closeness. A description of these three scales is provided in Appendix A. The choice of these three scales was based on several factors. First, the Social Potency scale corresponds well with the Extroversion dimension of the Big Five and, as mentioned in the introduction, the Extroversion dimension showed the highest meta-analytically derived correlation with leadership (.31). Second, Judge, et al. (2002) showed that the lower-order dominance trait of the Extroversion dimension showed a relatively high (.37) correlation against leadership criteria (see Table 3 in Judge, et al., 2002). Similarly, the MPQ Achievement scale corresponds to the lower order personality trait of achievement also shown by Judge, et al. (2002) to be highly correlated (.35) against leadership criteria. In addition, the MPQ Achievement scale is indicated as being rationally and empirically similar to the Conscientiousness factor in the Five Factor model (see Hough & Ones [2002] and Table 3 in Church [1994]). This Conscientiousness dimension also shows a relatively high meta-analytic correlation against leadership (.29) according to the Judge et al (2002) study. Finally, the Social Closeness MPQ scale empirically maps onto the Extroversion dimension of the Five Factor model (see Table 3, Church, 1994) and is conceptually Genetics and Leadership 13 similar to the lower order personality trait of sociability shown by Judge et al. (2002) to be correlated against leadership criteria (.24). The correlations between these three MPQ scales ranged from -.07 to .35 and were thus relatively independent of each other. Based on these previous findings, we predict these three MPQ scales to be significantly correlated against our measure of leadership emergence. Cognitive Measure: The verbal component of the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-R) was administered over the telephone to a random subset of the individuals as part of their earlier participation in the Minnesota Parenting Project. Like the MPQ, this test was administered several years prior to the present investigation. It should be noted that only a subsection of the sample had these data available (n=346). The mean for the Vocabulary score was 46.4 (s.d.=11.8); MZ’s scored significantly higher (m = 48.0) than DZ’s (m = 43.9) on this measure (F= 10.40, p< .01).. The means, standard deviations for the personality, cognitive, and leadership variables are shown in Table 3. Analytical Approach. As a first step in the analyses we correlated the various personality and cognitive variables against the leadership variable. These values are also shown in Table 3. It is clear from these single-order correlations that the leadership variable is significantly correlated against all four of these variables as predicted. The MPQ scale of Social Potency showed the highest correlation (.35) whereas the Social Closeness scale showed the lowest (.15). The multiple correlation coefficient (.40) between these variables and leadership was also significant at the .01 level but the Social Closeness variable did not exhibit a significant beta-weight (p <.40) and therefore this variable was dropped from further analyses. Genetics and Leadership 14 The second step in our analyses was to estimate the amount of variance in the various measures due to genetic and environmental components. Our quantitative genetic model is based on the assumption that the observed phenotypic variance (Vp) is a linear additive function of genetic (Vg) and shared (Vs) and non-shared (Vn) environmental variance, respectively. Symbolically, Vp = Vg + Vs + Vn. Under this model, the non-shared environmental variance represents residual variance not explained by either of the other two sources, confounded with measurement error. The expected covariance between any two members of a twin pair as a function of the variance components given above can be specified as, COV(MZ) =Vg + Vs COV(DZ) = .5*Vg + Vs Vg, or the proportion of the total variance attributable to additive genetic sources, can therefore expressed as: Vg = 2[Cov(MZ)-Cov(DZ)]. Or alternatively as: 2(MZ intraclass correlation-DZ intraclass correlation) where intraclass correlations are calculated separately for the monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins using the formula: (MSB-MSW)/(MSB + MSW), (1) Genetics and Leadership 15 and MSB is the Mean Square Between twin pairs and MSW is the Mean Square Within twin pairs from a One-way Analysis of Variance with twin pairs as the independent variable. Heritability estimates are based on several assumptions. The first is that twins (both MZ and DZ) are representative of the population as a whole for the trait in question. For personality traits, in particular as measured by the MPQ, this appears to be the case (Johnson, Krueger, Bouchard, & McGue, 2002). Second, we assume that MZ twins share environmental influences to the same degree as DZ twins. Several attempts have been made to uncover circumstances in which this assumption does not hold, with generally negative results. We attempted to test the validity of this assumption in this sample. A portion of the sample had completed a questionnaire assessing the degree of closeness of their relationship with their co-twin. For the MZ pairs that provided these data, the closeness of the twin relationship was not related to the twin similarity on the leadership variables. Another assumption is that there is no assortative mating (meaning that the parents of the twins were not similar) for the traits in question. When present, assortative mating tends to increase DZ twin correlations, but has no effect on Mz twin correlations. Thus, because some assortative mating (with coefficients ranging from .10 to .20) exists for most personality-related traits (Price & Vandenberg, 1980), this assumption generally has the effect of understating heritability estimates. Such relatively small values, however, are unlikely to reduce estimates of genetic influence substantially. We also assume that there are no genetic and environmental interactions. Again, though the concept of such interactions has great intuitive appeal, few replicable genotypeGenetics and Leadership 16 environment interactions have been found (Plomin, DeFries, & McClearn, 1988), although progress is being made in terms of detecting such interactions (Rutter & Silberg, 2002; Legrand, McGue, & Iacono, 1999). The final assumption is that the genetic variance is additive in the sense that if multiple genes influence the trait, they do so independently of each other. Violations of this assumption could mask shared environmental effects in reared-together twin studies, but without reared-apart twins, we have no way of addressing the extent to which the assumption holds in the current study. For the purposes of conducting our various genetic analyses we wished to remove skew from the leadership variable and therefore performed a log-transformation of this variable, re-standardized it, and multiplied it by 100. To place the personality and cognitive measures on the same scale as the leadership variable, they were likewise standardized and multiplied by 100. Table 4 shows the twin intraclass correlations for the personality and leadership measures, along with heritabilities they imply. As reported in other studies, the MPQ scales we used showed strong heritabilities. Of note is that the leadership variable demonstrated an implied heritability of .55, a considerably high value. At this point of our analyses, there was fairly good evidence for the heritabilities of the various independent predictor variables in our model as well as for the leadership variable. However, more sophisticated analyses can be applied 1) to provide more accurate estimates of heritabilities, 2) to separate environmental influences into shared and non-shared environmental sources and 3) to provide more clear estimates of the extent to which there are common influences among the predictor and the leadership Genetics and Leadership 17 variables. To this end, we first estimated the genetic influences (along with estimates of the influences of shared and non-shared environmental factors) associated with each of the variables derived from our leadership model using standard structural equation modeling procedures and the method of maximum likelihood as operationalized in the software program Mx (Neale, 1994). Using the separate trait covariance structures for the MZ and DZ twins, the procedure models the influencing factors according to the assumptions outlined above. The basic model for twin data includes three factors that influence an observed measurement (phenotype) or latent variable: additive genetic effects (A), common environmental effects, i.e. influences shared by members of the same family (C), and non-shared environmental effects and/or error (E). As shown in equation 2, variance in a phenotype can be expressed as the sum of variance attributable to each of the three factors, A, C, and E., each weighted by a parameter (h, c, or e) that determines their relative influence: Vleadership = a + c + e (2) Figure 1 presents the path model used to describe the relationships among the variables for two individuals who are either MZ or DZ twins. This is the path model used for the present analyses and is found in Heath et al. (1989). The paths e, a, and c represent the relative influences of the non-shared environmental, additive genetic, and shared environmental latent variables on an observed or latent leadership variable. MZ twins share all their genetic material, thus the correlation of 1.0 between the additive genetic components of Twin 1 and Twin 2 of the MZ pair; DZ twins share, on average, one half of their genes so that the corresponding correlation is .5 for the DZ twins. The common environment between pair members of both twin sets is set at 1.0, reflecting the Genetics and Leadership 18 assumption of equal common environmental influence, whereas the path between the non-shared environmental factors for the twins is, by definition, specified as zero. The modeling procedure estimates the three proportions, and reports the degree to which the resulting parameter estimates fit the assumptions. Table 5 shows these results of these structural equation heritability analyses for the different variables in our model. Perhaps of greatest importance is to examine the heritability of the observed leadership variable. The heritiability was .47 (95% confidence interval .09-.58) for this variable. To evaluate model fit we reviewed both the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1983) and the RMSEA. The AIC=Chisquare statistic-2df provides a summary index of both model fit and parsimony; models that have large negative values of AIC are preferred over those with smaller negative or positive AIC values. As can be seen, the univariate model fit particularly well for the leadership variable—the RMSEA was .00 and the AIC value was negative. Thus, there is good evidence for genetic influence for this observed variable. Also, the variance accounted for by the shared environmental factors was estimated as zero, whereas the non-shared environmental and/or measurement error factor accounted for the remaining amount of the variance (.53) for this leadership variable. Also, as shown in Table 5, there is good evidence for the heritabilities of the two personality variables as well (above .40) and the values observed are quite close to those obtained by Tellegen et al. (1988) for these specific variables. Also, of note is that the shared environment played almost no role in accounting for the variance for these variables as well. The genetic influence for the Vocabulary variable was .43, a value somewhat lower than what might be expected given the extensive research on the Genetics and Leadership 19 heritability of this variable. This lower value might be due to a more restricted range on this variable compared to a full-scale IQ measure. The proportion of variance due to the shared environment component was .28 for Vocabulary, which is also slightly deviant from previous research findings showing lower or no contribution of this component. The fit statistics for this particular variable were not strong either. The results from the structural equation models presented thus far only provide estimates of the variance in each particular measure associated with genetic and environmental factors. We are also interested in the extent to which the relationship between each of the predictors and leadership is genetically or environmentally mediated. To determine the later, multivariate models are needed. These multivariate models are direct generalizations of the univariate models, and allow estimates of the extent to which two measures share genetic influences (the genetic correlation), shared environmental influences (the shared environmental correlation), and non-shared environmental influences (the non-shared environmental correlation). To fit this model, we made use of the expectation-maximization algorithm to estimate MZ and DZ variance-covariance matrices. This procedure relies on maximum likelihood estimation procedures to generate the variance-covariance matrices making use of all available data, whether or not the data are complete for each individual. This method relies on the assumption that whatever data are missing are missing at random in the sense of the Little and Rubin (1987) discussion. We believe that this assumption was defensible because the process that generated the missing data (i.e., different questionnaires administered to different portions of the sample at different times) had nothing to do with the variables being observed and because there were no significant Genetics and Leadership 20 differences on other variables used in our study between those individuals for whom cognitive data were collected and those who did not have such data. The results of our analyses revealed that because the maximum likelihood surface was relatively flat, there were two solutions with similar fit statistics. (Chi-square values of 23.2 and 22.9 with 52 df, p=.99.). We present the solution (Chi-square = 23.2) that provided heritability estimates that most closely resembled those obtained from the above univariate analyses. However, the proportion of variance estimates and the genetic correlations from both solutions were similar. Table 6 shows the proportions of variance attributable to genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental sources for each of the traits based on this model. The fit statistics for this model were: AIC = -80.80 and the RMSEA = .000 indicating a very good fit. Again, the proportion of variance attributed to genetic influences for the leadership variable was relatively high (.39). As with the univariate model, no variance is attributed to the shared environment component. In some contrast with the univariate analysis, the proportion of variance attributed to genetic influences on vocabulary was only .39, but no variance was attributed to the shared environment. This finding is more consistent with previous research findings. Similar values were observed for the two personality variables, though the estimate of genetic influence is still relatively low. The structural equation analyses also generated several interesting correlational results. First, the modeled phenotypic (or observed) correlations of the personality and cognitive factors with the leadership variable was .36 for Social Potency, .23 for Achievement, and .06 for the vocabulary variable, indicating that the personality factors Genetics and Leadership 21 were relatively more important than the cognitive factor in their association with the leadership variable. Second, the genetic correlations between the various personality and cognitive variables and the latent leadership variable were generated. The genetic correlation reflects the extent to which whatever genetic variance is associated with two variables is likely to be in common. The squared value of a genetic correlation represents the proportion of the heritability of the latent leadership measure that can be explained by the genetic factors affecting each of the personality and cognitive factors. Consider two completely heritable traits such as eye and hair color. Though heritability is 100% for each, the genetic correlation is much lower, though it may not in fact be zero, as dark eyes and hair tend to be found in the same person, as do blue eyes and blonde hair. These data indicate that a substantial amount of the genetic influence on the leadership variable was common to the personality variables. The genetic correlation between the Social Potency and Leadership variable pair was .61 indicating that 37 percent of the genetic variance for leadership is shared or in common with that of the Social Potency. The genetic correlation between the Achievement and Leadership variable pair was .42, indicating that .17 percent of the genetic variance for leadership is shared with this personality factor. On the other hand, the genetic correlation for the Cognitive and Leadership pair was only .07, indicating that less than 1 percent of the genetic variance accounting for Leadership is in common with the Cognitive factor. One further question concerns the percentage of the genetic variance in leadership that is due to both personality and cognitive ability jointly. To estimate this, one can add the proportions of variance to the extent to which they are independent. Achievement Genetics and Leadership 22 and Vocabulary and Vocabulary and Social Potency are virtually independent (correlations are .01 and .07 respectively) and Social Potency and Vocabulary are correlated .18, so their common variance is 3%. Accordingly, the percentage of the genetic variance due to both personality and vocabulary would be 37% from Social Poise, plus 17% from Achievement, plus 1% from Vocabulary minus the common variances for a total of 53%. This is an approximate value, however. One of the objectives of the present research was to investigate the degree to which personality and cognitive variables would mediate any genetic influence observed. These data suggest that the relationship between the personality and leadership variables is mediated through a common genetic mechanism to some degree, but that genetic influences on cognitive ability and leadership are independent. The genetic influences on leadership are mainly dispersed amongst the two different personality factors but a substantial amount of genetic influence on leadership operates through other mechanisms and variables. Finally, because the proportion of variance accounted for by shared environmental factors for variables were estimated to be zero, there can be no shared environmental correlation, or it would be based on trace variances; we therefore do not report or comment on these values. The non-shared environmental correlations were likewise quite low (equal to or below .05).

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

The determinants of leadership role occupancy: Genetic and personality factors

This study investigated the influence of genetic factor and personality on leadership role occupancy among a sample of male twins. Identical twins (n =238) who share 100% of their genetic background were compared with fraternal twins (n =188) who are expected to share only 50% of their genetic background. Results indicated that 30% of the variance in leadership role occupancy could be accounted...

متن کامل

Develop a leadership model based on early maladaptive schemas and psychological flexibility

Background and Aim: Today, human resources are the most valuable asset of the organization.The entry of psychology into the management leap led to the use of various theories of psychology and citation and the creation of human relationships to explain the personality and behavior of the individual in the organization, and concepts such as motivation and organizational behavior were emphasized....

متن کامل

The Impact of Authentic Leadership on Risk Perception of Employees with the Mediating Role of Safe Climate in Ilam Petrochemical Company

This study investigated the impact of of authentic leadership on risk perception of employees with the mediating role of safe climate in Ilam Petrochemical Company. The present study is considered a descriptive survey in terms of purpose, and applied in terms of data collection. The statistical population consisted of Ilam Petrochemical Company's official and contract staff with about 700 perso...

متن کامل

A causal model of the relationship between cognitive processes and effective leadership of academic department chairs in Mashhad University of Medical Sciences

The present study was conducted aiming to investigate the causal relationship between cognitive processes and effective leadership of academic departments’ chairs in Mashhad University of Medical Sciences. Using descriptive correlational study method, 40 academic department chairs in Mashhad University of Medical Sciences were studied by census method. Data were collected through computer-based...

متن کامل

The relationship among transformational and transactional leadership styles and personality of Physical education (P.E) faculty manager`s in Iranian universities

The purpose of this study was to investigate the relationship between transformational and transactional leadership styles with the personality of Physical education (P.E) faculty manager`s in Iranian universities. Research method was a correlation and the statistical population (N=64) included all the managers and assistants of Physical Education Colleges in Iran. Data collection tool was the ...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2003